» Articles » PMID: 26072278

Imitate or Innovate? Children's Innovation is Influenced by the Efficacy of Observed Behaviour

Overview
Journal Cognition
Publisher Elsevier
Specialty Psychology
Date 2015 Jun 15
PMID 26072278
Citations 18
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

This study investigated the age at which children judge it futile to imitate unreliable information, in the form of a visibly ineffective demonstrated solution, and deviate to produce novel solutions ('innovations'). Children aged 4-9 years were presented with a novel puzzle box, the Multiple-Methods Box (MMB), which offered multiple innovation opportunities to extract a reward using different tools, access points and exits. 209 children were assigned to conditions in which eight social demonstrations of a reward retrieval method were provided; each condition differed incrementally in terms of the method's efficacy (0%, 25%, 75%, and 100% success at extracting the reward). An additional 47 children were assigned to a no-demonstration control condition. Innovative reward extractions from the MMB increased with decreasing efficacy of the demonstrated method. However, imitation remained a widely used strategy irrespective of the efficacy of the method being reproduced (90% of children produced at least one imitative attempt, and imitated on an average of 4.9 out of 8 attempt trials). Children were more likely to innovate in relation to the tool than exit, even though the latter would have been more effective. Overall, innovation was rare: only 12.4% of children innovated by discovering at least one novel reward exit. Children's prioritisation of social information is consistent with theories of cultural evolution indicating imitation is a prepotent response following observation of behaviour, and that innovation is a rarity; so much so, that even maladaptive behaviour is copied.

Citing Articles

Innovation through neurodiversity: Diversity is beneficial.

Axbey H, Beckmann N, Fletcher-Watson S, Tullo A, Crompton C Autism. 2023; 27(7):2193-2198.

PMID: 36880441 PMC: 10504802. DOI: 10.1177/13623613231158685.


3-4-year-old children's memory flexibility allows adaptation to an altered context.

Liszkai-Peres K, Kampis D, Kiraly I PLoS One. 2022; 17(9):e0275071.

PMID: 36149884 PMC: 9506616. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0275071.


Cognitive flexibility supports the development of cumulative cultural learning in children.

Davis S, Rawlings B, Clegg J, Ikejimba D, Watson-Jones R, Whiten A Sci Rep. 2022; 12(1):14073.

PMID: 35982124 PMC: 9388526. DOI: 10.1038/s41598-022-18231-7.


Taking account of others' goals in social information use: Developmental changes in 3- to 7-year-old children.

Blakey K, Atkinson M, Rafetseder E, Renner E, Caldwell C J Exp Child Psychol. 2021; 215:105325.

PMID: 34896763 PMC: 8784822. DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105325.


A New Look at Infant Problem-Solving: Using DeepLabCut to Investigate Exploratory Problem-Solving Approaches.

Solby H, Radovanovic M, Sommerville J Front Psychol. 2021; 12:705108.

PMID: 34819894 PMC: 8606407. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.705108.