» Articles » PMID: 25929653

Infant Motor Development and Cognitive Performance in Early Old Age: the Helsinki Birth Cohort Study

Overview
Journal Age (Dordr)
Publisher Springer
Specialty Geriatrics
Date 2015 May 2
PMID 25929653
Citations 4
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Motor development and cognitive development in childhood have been found to be fundamentally interrelated, but less is known about the association extending over the life course. The aim of this study was to examine the association between early motor development and cognitive performance in early old age. From men and women belonging to the Helsinki Birth Cohort Study, who were born between 1934 and 1944 and resided in Finland in 1971, 1279 participated in cognitive performance tests (CogState®, version 3.0.5) between 2001 and 2006 at an average age of 64.2 years (SD 3.0). Of these, age at first walking extracted from child welfare clinic records was available for 398 participants. Longer reaction times in cognitive tasks measuring simple reaction time (SRT), choice reaction time (CRT), working memory (WM), divided attention (DA), and associated learning (AL) indicated poorer cognitive performance. Adjustment was made for sex, age at testing, father's occupational status and own highest attained education, and occupation in adulthood. Average age of learning to walk was 12.2 months (SD 2.1). After adjusting for covariates, earlier attainment of learning to walk was associated with shorter reaction times in cognitive performance tasks (SRT 10.32 % per month, 95 % CI 0.48-21.12, p = 0.039; CRT 14.17 % per month, 95 % CI 3.75-25.63, p = 0.007; WM 15.14 % per month, 95 % CI 4.95-26.32, p = 0.003). People who learned to walk earlier had better cognitive performance in early old age. The earlier attainment of motor skills may track over to early old age and possibly reflect greater cognitive reserve in older age.

Citing Articles

Associations between Gross and Fine Motor Skills, Physical Activity, Executive Function, and Academic Achievement: Longitudinal Findings from the UK Millennium Cohort Study.

Zhou Y, Tolmie A Brain Sci. 2024; 14(2).

PMID: 38391696 PMC: 10887312. DOI: 10.3390/brainsci14020121.


Age of walking and intellectual ability in autism spectrum disorder and other neurodevelopmental disorders: a population-based study.

Havdahl A, Farmer C, Schjolberg S, Oyen A, Suren P, Reichborn-Kjennerud T J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2020; 62(9):1070-1078.

PMID: 33369747 PMC: 8236490. DOI: 10.1111/jcpp.13369.


Preserved expressive language as a phenotypic determinant of Mosaic Angelman Syndrome.

Carson R, Bird L, Childers A, Wheeler F, Duis J Mol Genet Genomic Med. 2019; 7(9):e837.

PMID: 31400086 PMC: 6732290. DOI: 10.1002/mgg3.837.


Predictive validity of developmental milestones for detecting limited intellectual functioning.

Vlasblom E, Boere-Boonekamp M, Hafkamp-de Groen E, Dusseldorp E, van Dommelen P, Verkerk P PLoS One. 2019; 14(3):e0214475.

PMID: 30921424 PMC: 6438572. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0214475.

References
1.
Wang L, van Belle G, Kukull W, Larson E . Predictors of functional change: a longitudinal study of nondemented people aged 65 and older. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2002; 50(9):1525-34. DOI: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2002.50408.x. View

2.
Haier R, Jung R, Yeo R, Head K, Alkire M . Structural brain variation and general intelligence. Neuroimage. 2004; 23(1):425-33. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.04.025. View

3.
Darby D, Maruff P, Collie A, McStephen M . Mild cognitive impairment can be detected by multiple assessments in a single day. Neurology. 2002; 59(7):1042-6. DOI: 10.1212/wnl.59.7.1042. View

4.
Royall D, Palmer R, Chiodo L, Polk M . Declining executive control in normal aging predicts change in functional status: the Freedom House Study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2004; 52(3):346-52. DOI: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2004.52104.x. View

5.
Gale C, OCallaghan F, Godfrey K, Law C, Martyn C . Critical periods of brain growth and cognitive function in children. Brain. 2003; 127(Pt 2):321-9. DOI: 10.1093/brain/awh034. View