Bruckner R, Heekeren H, Nassar M
Commun Psychol. 2025; 3(1):24.
PMID: 39948273
PMC: 11825852.
DOI: 10.1038/s44271-025-00203-y.
Leung A, Yurovsky D, Hawkins R
Child Dev. 2024; 96(2):546-561.
PMID: 39552570
PMC: 11868688.
DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14186.
Lapidow E, Bonawitz E
Open Mind (Camb). 2023; 7:855-878.
PMID: 37946850
PMC: 10631797.
DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00110.
Kolvoort I, Temme N, Van Maanen L
Open Mind (Camb). 2023; 7:318-349.
PMID: 37416078
PMC: 10320818.
DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00080.
Amir O, Tyomkin L, Hart Y
PLoS Comput Biol. 2022; 18(8):e1010358.
PMID: 35947588
PMC: 9394844.
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1010358.
Spontaneous discovery of novel task solutions in children.
Schuck N, Li A, Wenke D, Ay-Bryson D, Loewe A, Gaschler R
PLoS One. 2022; 17(5):e0266253.
PMID: 35639714
PMC: 9154107.
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0266253.
Theory Before the Test: How to Build High-Verisimilitude Explanatory Theories in Psychological Science.
van Rooij I, Baggio G
Perspect Psychol Sci. 2021; 16(4):682-697.
PMID: 33404356
PMC: 8273840.
DOI: 10.1177/1745691620970604.
Balancing exploration and exploitation with information and randomization.
Wilson R, Bonawitz E, Costa V, Ebitz R
Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2020; 38:49-56.
PMID: 33184605
PMC: 7654823.
DOI: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2020.10.001.
Eye movements reflect adaptive predictions and predictive precision.
Bakst L, McGuire J
J Exp Psychol Gen. 2020; 150(5):915-929.
PMID: 33048566
PMC: 8039063.
DOI: 10.1037/xge0000977.
Childhood as a solution to explore-exploit tensions.
Gopnik A
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2020; 375(1803):20190502.
PMID: 32475327
PMC: 7293160.
DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2019.0502.
Young children combine sensory cues with learned information in a statistically efficient manner: But task complexity matters.
Bejjanki V, Randrup E, Aslin R
Dev Sci. 2019; 23(3):e12912.
PMID: 31608526
PMC: 7153990.
DOI: 10.1111/desc.12912.
Asking the right questions about the psychology of human inquiry: Nine open challenges.
Coenen A, Nelson J, Gureckis T
Psychon Bull Rev. 2018; 26(5):1548-1587.
PMID: 29869025
DOI: 10.3758/s13423-018-1470-5.
Changes in cognitive flexibility and hypothesis search across human life history from childhood to adolescence to adulthood.
Gopnik A, OGrady S, Lucas C, Griffiths T, Wente A, Bridgers S
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2017; 114(30):7892-7899.
PMID: 28739917
PMC: 5544286.
DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1700811114.
The anchoring bias reflects rational use of cognitive resources.
Lieder F, Griffiths T, Huys Q, Goodman N
Psychon Bull Rev. 2017; 25(1):322-349.
PMID: 28484952
DOI: 10.3758/s13423-017-1286-8.
Two- and three-year-olds track a single meaning during word learning: Evidence for Propose-but-verify.
Woodard K, Gleitman L, Trueswell J
Lang Learn Dev. 2016; 12(3):252-261.
PMID: 27672354
PMC: 5035104.
DOI: 10.1080/15475441.2016.1140581.
Learning Probabilistic Inference through Spike-Timing-Dependent Plasticity.
Pecevski D, Maass W
eNeuro. 2016; 3(2).
PMID: 27419214
PMC: 4916275.
DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0048-15.2016.
Structure Learning in Bayesian Sensorimotor Integration.
Genewein T, Hez E, Razzaghpanah Z, Braun D
PLoS Comput Biol. 2015; 11(8):e1004369.
PMID: 26305797
PMC: 4549275.
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004369.
The Sense of Confidence during Probabilistic Learning: A Normative Account.
Meyniel F, Schlunegger D, Dehaene S
PLoS Comput Biol. 2015; 11(6):e1004305.
PMID: 26076466
PMC: 4468157.
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004305.