» Articles » PMID: 23672476

The Idea of an Exact Number: Children's Understanding of Cardinality and Equinumerosity

Overview
Journal Cogn Sci
Specialty Psychology
Date 2013 May 16
PMID 23672476
Citations 9
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Understanding what numbers are means knowing several things. It means knowing how counting relates to numbers (called the cardinal principle or cardinality); it means knowing that each number is generated by adding one to the previous number (called the successor function or succession), and it means knowing that all and only sets whose members can be placed in one-to-one correspondence have the same number of items (called exact equality or equinumerosity). A previous study (Sarnecka & Carey, 2008) linked children's understanding of cardinality to their understanding of succession for the numbers five and six. This study investigates the link between cardinality and equinumerosity for these numbers, finding that children either understand both cardinality and equinumerosity or they understand neither. This suggests that cardinality and equinumerosity (along with succession) are interrelated facets of the concepts five and six, the acquisition of which is an important conceptual achievement of early childhood.

Citing Articles

When eleven does not equal 11: Investigating exactness at a number's upper bound.

Noveck I, Fogel M, Voorhees K, Turco G PLoS One. 2022; 17(4):e0266920.

PMID: 35482732 PMC: 9049330. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0266920.


Numerical Training Videos and Early Numerical Achievement: A Study on 3-Year-Old Preschoolers.

Cuder A, Vidoz M, De Vita C, Pellizzoni S, Passolunghi M Brain Sci. 2022; 12(1).

PMID: 35053831 PMC: 8773724. DOI: 10.3390/brainsci12010088.


What's in a Hub?-Representing Identity in Language and Mathematics.

Arora A, Pletzer B, Aichhorn M, Perner J Neuroscience. 2020; 432:104-114.

PMID: 32112913 PMC: 7100012. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2020.02.032.


One-to-one correspondence without language.

Koopman S, Arre A, Piantadosi S, Cantlon J R Soc Open Sci. 2019; 6(10):190495.

PMID: 31824689 PMC: 6837223. DOI: 10.1098/rsos.190495.


Configured-groups hypothesis: fast comparison of exact large quantities without counting.

Miravete S, Tricot A, Kalyuga S, Amadieu F Cogn Process. 2017; 18(4):447-459.

PMID: 28717829 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-017-0826-5.


References
1.
Davidson K, Eng K, Barner D . Does learning to count involve a semantic induction?. Cognition. 2012; 123(1):162-73. DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.12.013. View

2.
Condry K, Spelke E . The development of language and abstract concepts: the case of natural number. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2008; 137(1):22-38. DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.137.1.22. View

3.
Sarnecka B, Carey S . How counting represents number: what children must learn and when they learn it. Cognition. 2008; 108(3):662-74. DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.05.007. View

4.
Halberda J, Feigenson L . Developmental change in the acuity of the "Number Sense": The Approximate Number System in 3-, 4-, 5-, and 6-year-olds and adults. Dev Psychol. 2008; 44(5):1457-65. DOI: 10.1037/a0012682. View

5.
Wynn K . Children's understanding of counting. Cognition. 1990; 36(2):155-93. DOI: 10.1016/0010-0277(90)90003-3. View