» Articles » PMID: 23545385

Interaction Between Language and Vision: It's Momentary, Abstract, and It Develops

Overview
Journal Cognition
Publisher Elsevier
Specialty Psychology
Date 2013 Apr 3
PMID 23545385
Citations 12
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

In this paper, we present a case study that explores the nature and development of the mechanisms by which language interacts with and influences our ability to represent and retain information from one of our most important non-linguistic systems - vision. In previous work (Dessalegn & Landau, 2008), we showed that 4 year-olds remembered conjunctions of visual features better when the visual target was accompanied by a sentence containing an asymmetric spatial predicate (e.g., the yellow is to the left of the black) but not when the visual target was accompanied by a sentence containing a novel noun (e.g., look at the dax) or a symmetric spatial predicate (e.g., the yellow is touching the black). In this paper, we extend these findings. In three experiments, 3, 4 and 6 year-olds were shown square blocks split in half by color vertically, horizontally or diagonally (e.g., yellow-left, black-right) and were asked to perform a delayed-matching task. We found that sentences containing spatial asymmetric predicates (e.g., the yellow is to the left of the black) and non-spatial asymmetric predicates (e.g., the yellow is prettier than the black) helped 4 year-olds, although not to the same extent. By contrast, 3 year-olds did not benefit from different linguistic instructions at all while 6 year-olds performed at ceiling in the task with or without the relevant sentences. Our findings suggest by age 4, the effects of language on non-linguistic tasks depend on highly abstract representations of the linguistic instructions and are momentary, seen only in the context of the task. We further speculate that language becomes more automatically engaged in nonlinguistic tasks over development.

Citing Articles

What have we learned from research on the "geometric module"?.

Newcombe N Learn Behav. 2023; 52(1):14-18.

PMID: 38030808 DOI: 10.3758/s13420-023-00617-w.


Different reference frames on different axes: Space and language in indigenous Amazonians.

Pitt B, Carstensen A, Boni I, Piantadosi S, Gibson E Sci Adv. 2022; 8(47):eabp9814.

PMID: 36427312 PMC: 9699666. DOI: 10.1126/sciadv.abp9814.


Examining the role of external language support and children's own language use in spatial development.

Miller-Goldwater H, Simmering V J Exp Child Psychol. 2021; 215:105317.

PMID: 34920377 PMC: 8748416. DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105317.


Spatial Language of Young Children During Block Play in Kindergartens in Urban China.

Yang X, Pan Y Front Psychol. 2021; 12:568638.

PMID: 33708154 PMC: 7940534. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.568638.


Using eye-tracking to understand relations between visual attention and language in children's spatial skills.

Miller H, Kirkorian H, Simmering V Cogn Psychol. 2020; 117:101264.

PMID: 31901602 PMC: 7181305. DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2019.101264.


References
1.
Papafragou A, Hulbert J, Trueswell J . Does language guide event perception? Evidence from eye movements. Cognition. 2008; 108(1):155-84. PMC: 2810627. DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.02.007. View

2.
Treisman A, Schmidt H . Illusory conjunctions in the perception of objects. Cogn Psychol. 1982; 14(1):107-41. DOI: 10.1016/0010-0285(82)90006-8. View

3.
Hagen J, Kingsley P . Labeling effects in short-term memory. Child Dev. 1968; 39(1):113-21. View

4.
Frank M, Fedorenko E, Lai P, Saxe R, Gibson E . Verbal interference suppresses exact numerical representation. Cogn Psychol. 2011; 64(1-2):74-92. DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2011.10.004. View

5.
Davidoff J, Davies I, Roberson D . Colour categories in a stone-age tribe. Nature. 1999; 398(6724):203-4. DOI: 10.1038/18335. View