» Articles » PMID: 23532909

Perception of Second- and Third-order Orientation Signals and Their Interactions

Overview
Journal J Vis
Specialty Ophthalmology
Date 2013 Mar 28
PMID 23532909
Citations 16
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Orientation signals, which are crucial to many aspects of visual function, are more complex and varied in the natural world than in the stimuli typically used for laboratory investigation. Gratings and lines have a single orientation, but in natural stimuli, local features have multiple orientations, and multiple orientations can occur even at the same location. Moreover, orientation cues can arise not only from pairwise spatial correlations, but from higher-order ones as well. To investigate these orientation cues and how they interact, we examined segmentation performance for visual textures in which the strengths of different kinds of orientation cues were varied independently, while controlling potential confounds such as differences in luminance statistics. Second-order cues (the kind present in gratings) at different orientations are largely processed independently: There is no cancellation of positive and negative signals at orientations that differ by 45°. Third-order orientation cues are readily detected and interact only minimally with second-order cues. However, they combine across orientations in a different way: Positive and negative signals largely cancel if the orientations differ by 90°. Two additional elements are superimposed on this picture. First, corners play a special role. When second-order orientation cues combine to produce corners, they provide a stronger signal for texture segregation than can be accounted for by their individual effects. Second, while the object versus background distinction does not influence processing of second-order orientation cues, this distinction influences the processing of third-order orientation cues.

Citing Articles

Low sensitivity for orientation in texture similarity ratings.

Nothdurft H J Vis. 2024; 24(13):14.

PMID: 39705048 PMC: 11668350. DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.13.14.


Discrimination of textures with spatial correlations and multiple gray levels.

Victor J, Rizvi S, Bush J, Conte M J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis. 2023; 40(2):237-258.

PMID: 36821194 PMC: 9971653. DOI: 10.1364/JOSAA.472553.


Functional recursion of orientation cues in figure-ground separation.

Victor J, Conte M Vision Res. 2022; 197:108047.

PMID: 35691090 PMC: 9262819. DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2022.108047.


Redundancy between spectral and higher-order texture statistics for natural image segmentation.

Herrera-Esposito D, Gomez-Sena L, Coen-Cagli R Vision Res. 2021; 187:55-65.

PMID: 34217005 PMC: 8363589. DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2021.06.007.


Flexible contextual modulation of naturalistic texture perception in peripheral vision.

Herrera-Esposito D, Coen-Cagli R, Gomez-Sena L J Vis. 2021; 21(1):1.

PMID: 33393962 PMC: 7794279. DOI: 10.1167/jov.21.1.1.


References
1.
Victor J, Conte M . Local image statistics: maximum-entropy constructions and perceptual salience. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis. 2012; 29(7):1313-45. PMC: 3396046. DOI: 10.1364/JOSAA.29.001313. View

2.
Deneve S, Latham P, Pouget A . Reading population codes: a neural implementation of ideal observers. Nat Neurosci. 1999; 2(8):740-5. DOI: 10.1038/11205. View

3.
Poirson A, Wandell B, Varner D, Brainard D . Surface characterizations of color thresholds. J Opt Soc Am A. 1990; 7(4):783-9. DOI: 10.1364/josaa.7.000783. View

4.
Sagi D . The combination of spatial frequency and orientation is effortlessly perceived. Percept Psychophys. 1988; 43(6):601-3. DOI: 10.3758/bf03207749. View

5.
Victor J, Conte M . Visual working memory for image statistics. Vision Res. 2003; 44(6):541-56. DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2003.11.001. View