» Articles » PMID: 22642356

Age-related Differences in Emotional Reactivity, Regulation, and Rejection Sensitivity in Adolescence

Overview
Journal Emotion
Specialty Psychology
Date 2012 May 31
PMID 22642356
Citations 162
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Although adolescents' emotional lives are thought to be more turbulent than those of adults, it is unknown whether this difference is attributable to developmental changes in emotional reactivity or emotion regulation. Study 1 addressed this question by presenting healthy individuals aged 10-23 with negative and neutral pictures and asking them to respond naturally or use cognitive reappraisal to down-regulate their responses on a trial-by-trial basis. Results indicated that age exerted both linear and quadratic effects on regulation success but was unrelated to emotional reactivity. Study 2 replicated and extended these findings using a different reappraisal task and further showed that situational (i.e., social vs. nonsocial stimuli) and dispositional (i.e., level of rejection sensitivity) social factors interacted with age to predict regulation success: young adolescents were less successful at regulating responses to social than to nonsocial stimuli, particularly if the adolescents were high in rejection sensitivity. Taken together, these results have important implications for the inclusion of emotion regulation in models of emotional and cognitive development.

Citing Articles

Relationship Between Schizotypal Traits, Emotion Regulation, and Negative Affect in Children: A Network Analysis.

Ren Q, Yang T, Wang Y, Lui S, Chan R Schizophr Bull. 2025; 51(Supplement_2):S226-S237.

PMID: 40037824 PMC: 11879503. DOI: 10.1093/schbul/sbae172.


Kindness Is the Language That the Deaf Can Hear and the Blind Can See: Kindness, Theory of Mind and Well-Being in Adolescents.

Stamp P, Bosacki S, Talwar V Children (Basel). 2025; 11(12.

PMID: 39767984 PMC: 11726870. DOI: 10.3390/children11121555.


Links between self-regulation patterns and prosocial behavior trajectories from middle childhood to early adolescence: a longitudinal study.

Ritgens C, Bondu R, Warschburger P Front Psychol. 2024; 15:1480046.

PMID: 39737226 PMC: 11684097. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1480046.


Cognitive Profile Discrepancy as a Possible Predictor of Emotion Dysregulation in a Clinical Sample of Female Adolescents with Suicidal Behavior.

Furente F, Annecchini F, Matera E, Serafino S, Frigeri G, Gabellone A Eur J Investig Health Psychol Educ. 2024; 14(12):3087-3098.

PMID: 39727510 PMC: 11675136. DOI: 10.3390/ejihpe14120202.


Emotion Regulation Moderates the Prospective Association between ERN and Anxiety in Early Adolescence: An Age-Specific Moderation of Cognitive Reappraisal but not Expressive Suppression.

Tan J, Liu P Res Child Adolesc Psychopathol. 2024; 53(2):261-277.

PMID: 39585576 DOI: 10.1007/s10802-024-01263-0.


References
1.
Prencipe A, Kesek A, Cohen J, Lamm C, Lewis M, Zelazo P . Development of hot and cool executive function during the transition to adolescence. J Exp Child Psychol. 2010; 108(3):621-37. DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.09.008. View

2.
Chambers C, Johnston C . Developmental differences in children's use of rating scales. J Pediatr Psychol. 2001; 27(1):27-36. DOI: 10.1093/jpepsy/27.1.27. View

3.
Gardner M, Steinberg L . Peer influence on risk taking, risk preference, and risky decision making in adolescence and adulthood: an experimental study. Dev Psychol. 2005; 41(4):625-35. DOI: 10.1037/0012-1649.41.4.625. View

4.
Steinberg L, Morris A . Adolescent development. Annu Rev Psychol. 2001; 52:83-110. DOI: 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.83. View

5.
Wang L, Koch G . Secondary sexual characteristics in boys: estimates from the national health and nutrition examination survey III, 1988-1994. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2001; 155(9):1022-8. DOI: 10.1001/archpedi.155.9.1022. View