» Articles » PMID: 22442574

The Relative Importance of Retinal Error and Prediction in Saccadic Adaptation

Overview
Journal J Neurophysiol
Specialties Neurology
Physiology
Date 2012 Mar 24
PMID 22442574
Citations 40
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

When saccades systematically miss their visual target, their amplitude adjusts, causing the position errors to be progressively reduced. Conventionally, this adaptation is viewed as driven by retinal error (the distance between primary saccade endpoint and visual target). Recent work suggests that the oculomotor system is informed about where the eye lands; thus not all "retinal error" is unexpected. The present study compared two error signals that may drive saccade adaptation: retinal error and prediction error (the difference between predicted and actual postsaccadic images). Subjects made saccades to a visual target in two successive sessions. In the first session, the target was extinguished during saccade execution if the amplitude was smaller (or, in other experiments, greater) than the running median, thereby modifying the average retinal error subjects experienced without moving the target during the saccade as in conventional adaptation paradigms. In the second session, targets were extinguished at the start of saccades and turned back on at a position that reproduced the trial-by-trial retinal error recorded in the first session. Despite the retinal error in the first and second sessions having been identical, adaptation was severalfold greater in the second session, when the predicted target position had been changed. These results argue that the eye knows where it lands and where it expects the target to be, and that deviations from this prediction drive saccade adaptation more strongly than retinal error alone.

Citing Articles

Serial dependencies in motor targeting as a function of target appearance.

Tyralla S, Zimmermann E J Vis. 2024; 24(13):6.

PMID: 39641949 PMC: 11629911. DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.13.6.


No evidence for differential saccadic adaptation in children and adults with an autism spectrum diagnosis.

Tarrit K, Freedman E, Francisco A, Horsthuis D, Molholm S, Foxe J Front Integr Neurosci. 2023; 17:1232474.

PMID: 37869448 PMC: 10587467. DOI: 10.3389/fnint.2023.1232474.


Adaptive changes to saccade amplitude and target localization do not require pre-saccadic target visibility.

Heins F, Masselink J, Scherer J, Lappe M Sci Rep. 2023; 13(1):8315.

PMID: 37221275 PMC: 10205735. DOI: 10.1038/s41598-023-35434-8.


Motor recalibration of visual and saccadic maps.

Tyralla S, Pome A, Zimmermann E Proc Biol Sci. 2023; 290(1994):20222566.

PMID: 36855869 PMC: 9975659. DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2022.2566.


Interaction of dynamic error signals in saccade adaptation.

Wagner I, Schutz A J Neurophysiol. 2023; 129(3):717-732.

PMID: 36791071 PMC: 10027077. DOI: 10.1152/jn.00419.2022.


References
1.
Joiner W, FitzGibbon E, Wurtz R . Amplitudes and directions of individual saccades can be adjusted by corollary discharge. J Vis. 2010; 10(2):22.1-12. PMC: 2932478. DOI: 10.1167/10.2.22. View

2.
HALLETT P, Lightstone A . Saccadic eye movements towards stimuli triggered by prior saccades. Vision Res. 1976; 16(1):99-106. DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(76)90083-3. View

3.
Wolpert D, Miall R . Forward Models for Physiological Motor Control. Neural Netw. 1996; 9(8):1265-1279. DOI: 10.1016/s0893-6080(96)00035-4. View

4.
Deubel H, Schneider W, Bridgeman B . Postsaccadic target blanking prevents saccadic suppression of image displacement. Vision Res. 1996; 36(7):985-96. DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(95)00203-0. View

5.
Chen-Harris H, Joiner W, Ethier V, Zee D, Shadmehr R . Adaptive control of saccades via internal feedback. J Neurosci. 2008; 28(11):2804-13. PMC: 2733833. DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5300-07.2008. View