» Articles » PMID: 22110464

Reward and Punishment Effects on Error Processing and Conflict Control

Overview
Journal Front Psychol
Date 2011 Nov 24
PMID 22110464
Citations 38
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Recently, positive affect has been reported to reduce cognitive conflicts and adaptations related to conflict control. van Steenbergen et al. (2009) proposed that the aversive quality of conflicts drives short-term adaptations following a conflict. They reasoned that monetary gain and its positive emotional consequences might counteract the aversive quality of conflict and hence reduce subsequent adaptations. In two experiments, we combined Simon-type conflicts with monetary gains and losses in between trials and analyzed event-related brain potentials. In Experiment 1, gains and losses occurred randomly between trials as a lottery, whereas in Experiment 2 gains and losses were contingent upon performance, either rewarding the 25% fastest responses or penalizing the 25% slowest responses. In Experiment 1, conflict adaptation was completely unaffected by gains or losses; contrary to predictions, in Experiment 2, conflict adaptation in reward blocks was more pronounced after a gain. In Experiment 2 we also investigated the error-related negativity (ERN) - a brain signal proposed to be related to performance monitoring. The ERN and behavioral post-error slowing were enlarged in the context of reward; therefore, reward increases error adaptation, possibly by enhancing the subjective value of errors. In conclusion, affective modulations of conflict adaptations seem to be much more limited than previously asserted and adaptive mechanisms triggered by errors and conflicts dissociate.

Citing Articles

The impact of impulsivity and compulsivity on error processing in different motivational contexts.

Overmeyer R, Endrass T Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2025; .

PMID: 40045099 DOI: 10.3758/s13415-025-01281-5.


The influence of reward and loss outcomes after free- and forced-tasks on voluntary task choice.

Mittelstadt V, Mackenzie I, Leuthold H Psychol Res. 2024; 88(7):2059-2079.

PMID: 39078508 PMC: 11450031. DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02009-9.


Parsing the contributions of negative affect vs. aversive motivation to cognitive control: an experimental investigation.

Yang Q, Si S, Pourtois G Front Behav Neurosci. 2023; 17:1209824.

PMID: 37791110 PMC: 10543231. DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2023.1209824.


Performance-contingent reward increases the use of congruent distracting information.

Frober K, Lerche V Atten Percept Psychophys. 2023; 85(3):905-929.

PMID: 36918512 PMC: 10014142. DOI: 10.3758/s13414-023-02682-9.


Imbalanced weighting of proactive and reactive control as a marker of risk-taking propensity.

Grisetto F, Le Denmat P, Delevoye-Turrell Y, Vantrepotte Q, Davin T, Dinca A PLoS One. 2023; 18(1):e0277246.

PMID: 36662753 PMC: 9858370. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0277246.


References
1.
Praamstra P, Plat F . Failed suppression of direct visuomotor activation in Parkinson's disease. J Cogn Neurosci. 2001; 13(1):31-43. DOI: 10.1162/089892901564153. View

2.
Ridderinkhof K, Ullsperger M, Crone E, Nieuwenhuis S . The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science. 2004; 306(5695):443-7. DOI: 10.1126/science.1100301. View

3.
Olvet D, Hajcak G . The error-related negativity (ERN) and psychopathology: toward an endophenotype. Clin Psychol Rev. 2008; 28(8):1343-54. PMC: 2615243. DOI: 10.1016/j.cpr.2008.07.003. View

4.
Wiswede D, Munte T, Goschke T, Russeler J . Modulation of the error-related negativity by induction of short-term negative affect. Neuropsychologia. 2008; 47(1):83-90. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.08.016. View

5.
Holroyd C, Coles M . Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex integrates reinforcement history to guide voluntary behavior. Cortex. 2008; 44(5):548-59. DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2007.08.013. View