» Articles » PMID: 20976034

Putting Short-Term Memory Into Context: Reply to Usher, Davelaar, Haarmann, and Goshen-Gottstein (2008)

Overview
Journal Psychol Rev
Specialty Psychology
Date 2010 Oct 27
PMID 20976034
Citations 5
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

The temporal context model posits that search through episodic memory is driven by associations between the multiattribute representations of items and context. Context, in turn, is a recency weighted sum of previous experiences or memories. Because recently processed items are most similar to the current representation of context, M. Usher, E. J. Davelaar, H. J. Haarmann, and Y. Goshen-Gottstein (2008) have suggested that the temporal context model (TCM-A) embodies a distinction between short-term and long-term memory and that this distinction is central to TCM-A's success in accounting for the pattern of recency and contiguity observed across short and long timescales. The authors dispute Usher et al's claim that context in TCM-A has much in common with classic interpretations of short-term memory. The idea that multiple representations interact in the process of memory encoding and retrieval (across timescales), as proposed in TCM-A, is very different from the classic dual-process view that distinct rules govern retrieval of recent and remote memories.

Citing Articles

Explicit Sequence Memory in Recall of Temporally-structured Episodes.

Stern Y, Katz R, Sadeh T Sci Rep. 2020; 10(1):2666.

PMID: 32060365 PMC: 7021684. DOI: 10.1038/s41598-020-59472-8.


Long-Term Recency in Anterograde Amnesia.

Talmi D, Caplan J, Richards B, Moscovitch M PLoS One. 2015; 10(6):e0124084.

PMID: 26046770 PMC: 4457419. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0124084.


The conditional-recency dissociation is confounded with nominal recency: should unitary models of memory still be devaluated?.

Moran R, Goshen-Gottstein Y Psychon Bull Rev. 2013; 21(2):332-43.

PMID: 24030471 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-013-0508-y.


Constructing semantic representations from a gradually-changing representation of temporal context.

Howard M, Shankar K, Jagadisan U Top Cogn Sci. 2011; 3(1):48-73.

PMID: 21552364 PMC: 3086794. DOI: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01112.x.


A context maintenance and retrieval model of organizational processes in free recall.

Polyn S, Norman K, Kahana M Psychol Rev. 2009; 116(1):129-56.

PMID: 19159151 PMC: 2630591. DOI: 10.1037/a0014420.

References
1.
Marois R, Ivanoff J . Capacity limits of information processing in the brain. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005; 9(6):296-305. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.04.010. View

2.
Usher M, Davelaar E, Haarmann H, Goshen-Gottstein Y . Short-term memory after all: comment on Sederberg, Howard, and Kahana (2008). Psychol Rev. 2008; 115(4):1108-18. DOI: 10.1037/a0013725. View

3.
Jang Y, Huber D . Context retrieval and context change in free recall: recalling from long-term memory drives list isolation. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2008; 34(1):112-27. DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.34.1.112. View

4.
Nieuwenstein M, Potter M . Temporal limits of selection and memory encoding: A comparison of whole versus partial report in rapid serial visual presentation. Psychol Sci. 2006; 17(6):471-5. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01730.x. View

5.
Kahana M, Howard M, Zaromb F, Wingfield A . Age dissociates recency and lag recency effects in free recall. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2002; 28(3):530-40. DOI: 10.1037//0278-7393.28.3.530. View