» Articles » PMID: 20679113

The Interplay of Cognition and Cooperation

Overview
Specialty Biology
Date 2010 Aug 4
PMID 20679113
Citations 81
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Cooperation often involves behaviours that reduce immediate payoffs for actors. Delayed benefits have often been argued to pose problems for the evolution of cooperation because learning such contingencies may be difficult as partners may cheat in return. Therefore, the ability to achieve stable cooperation has often been linked to a species' cognitive abilities, which is in turn linked to the evolution of increasingly complex central nervous systems. However, in their famous 1981 paper, Axelrod and Hamilton stated that in principle even bacteria could play a tit-for-tat strategy in an iterated Prisoner's Dilemma. While to our knowledge this has not been documented, interspecific mutualisms are present in bacteria, plants and fungi. Moreover, many species which have evolved large brains in complex social environments lack convincing evidence in favour of reciprocity. What conditions must be fulfilled so that organisms with little to no brainpower, including plants and single-celled organisms, can, on average, gain benefits from interactions with partner species? On the other hand, what conditions favour the evolution of large brains and flexible behaviour, which includes the use of misinformation and so on? These questions are critical, as they begin to address why cognitive complexity would emerge when 'simple' cooperation is clearly sufficient in some cases. This paper spans the literature from bacteria to humans in our search for the key variables that link cooperation and deception to cognition.

Citing Articles

Higher eigenvector centrality in grooming network is linked to better inhibitory control task performance but not other cognitive tasks in free-ranging Japanese macaques.

Kaigaishi Y, Yamamoto S Sci Rep. 2024; 14(1):26804.

PMID: 39562645 PMC: 11577106. DOI: 10.1038/s41598-024-77912-7.


Development of a Marmoset Apparatus for Automated Pulling to study cooperative behaviors.

Meisner O, Shi W, Fagan N, Greenwood J, Jadi M, Nandy A Elife. 2024; 13.

PMID: 39466838 PMC: 11517257. DOI: 10.7554/eLife.97088.


Microbial markets: socio-economic perspective in studying microbial communities.

Mostafa F, Kruger A, Nies T, Frunzke J, Schipper K, Matuszynska A Microlife. 2024; 5:uqae016.

PMID: 39318452 PMC: 11421381. DOI: 10.1093/femsml/uqae016.


The paradigm of tax-reward and tax-punishment strategies in the advancement of public resource management dynamics.

Wang L, Liu Y, Guo R, Zhang L, Liu L, Hua S Proc Biol Sci. 2024; 291(2024):20240182.

PMID: 38864335 PMC: 11286137. DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2024.0182.


Collaborative hunting in artificial agents with deep reinforcement learning.

Tsutsui K, Tanaka R, Takeda K, Fujii K Elife. 2024; 13.

PMID: 38711355 PMC: 11076040. DOI: 10.7554/eLife.85694.


References
1.
de Quervain D, Fischbacher U, Treyer V, Schellhammer M, Schnyder U, Buck A . The neural basis of altruistic punishment. Science. 2004; 305(5688):1254-8. DOI: 10.1126/science.1100735. View

2.
Karavanich , Atema . Individual recognition and memory in lobster dominance. Anim Behav. 1999; 56(6):1553-1560. DOI: 10.1006/anbe.1998.0914. View

3.
Dolan R . Emotion, cognition, and behavior. Science. 2002; 298(5596):1191-4. DOI: 10.1126/science.1076358. View

4.
Jaeggi A, Burkart J, van Schaik C . On the psychology of cooperation in humans and other primates: combining the natural history and experimental evidence of prosociality. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2010; 365(1553):2723-35. PMC: 2936168. DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0118. View

5.
Beran M, Evans T . Maintenance of delay of gratification by four chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): the effects of delayed reward visibility, experimenter presence, and extended delay intervals. Behav Processes. 2006; 73(3):315-24. DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2006.07.005. View