» Articles » PMID: 20592955

Age Changes in Prosocial Responding and Moral Reasoning in Adolescence and Early Adulthood

Overview
Journal J Res Adolesc
Specialties Pediatrics
Psychology
Date 2011 Sep 28
PMID 20592955
Citations 83
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Age changes' measures of prosocial responding and reasoning were examined. Participants' reports of helping, empathy-related responding, and prosocial moral reasoning were obtained in adolescence (from age 15-16 years) and into adulthood (to age 25-26 years). Perspective taking and approval/interpersonal oriented/stereotypic prosocial moral reasoning increased from adolescence into adulthood, whereas personal distress declined. Helping declined and then increased (a cubic trend). Prosocial moral judgment composite scores (and self-reflective empathic reasoning) generally increased from late adolescence into the early 20s (age 17-18 to 21-22) but either leveled off or declined slightly thereafter (i.e., showed linear and cubic trends); rudimentary needs-oriented reasoning showed the reverse pattern of change. The increase in self-reflective empathic moral reasoning was for females only. Thus, perspective taking and some aspects of prosocial moral reasoning-capacities with a strong sociocognitive basis-showed the clearest increases with age, whereas simple prosocial proclivities (i.e., helping, sympathy) did not increase with age.

Citing Articles

Longitudinal Relations between Social Mindfulness and Social Behavior in Early Adolescence: Disentangling Between- and Within-Person Associations.

Yang L, Lyu M, Chen J, Feng X, Ren P J Youth Adolesc. 2025; .

PMID: 39945984 DOI: 10.1007/s10964-025-02142-7.


Emotional engagement with close friends in adolescence predicts neural correlates of empathy in adulthood.

Lin J, Stern J, Allen J, Boker S, Coan J Soc Neurosci. 2024; 19(4):246-258.

PMID: 39324514 PMC: 11620930. DOI: 10.1080/17470919.2024.2406863.


Oxytocin, but not vasopressin, decreases willingness to harm others by promoting moral emotions of guilt and shame.

Zheng X, Wang J, Yang X, Xu L, Becker B, Sahakian B Mol Psychiatry. 2024; 29(11):3475-3482.

PMID: 38769372 PMC: 11540849. DOI: 10.1038/s41380-024-02590-w.


Cooperative behavior in adolescents: a contribution of empathy and emotional regulation?.

Martinez-Velazquez E, Ponce-Juarez S, Diaz Furlong A, Sequeira H Front Psychol. 2024; 15:1342458.

PMID: 38638520 PMC: 11024332. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1342458.


The relationship between theory of mind and moral sensitivity among Chinese preschool children: the mediating role of empathy.

Du J, Liang Y, Guo D, Xiao Y BMC Psychol. 2024; 12(1):112.

PMID: 38429758 PMC: 10908148. DOI: 10.1186/s40359-024-01600-4.


References
1.
Mayseless O, Scharf M . What does it mean to be an adult? The Israeli experience. New Dir Child Adolesc Dev. 2003; (100):5-20. DOI: 10.1002/cd.71. View

2.
Eisenberg N, Guthrie I, Cumberland A, Murphy B, Shepard S, Zhou Q . Prosocial development in early adulthood: a longitudinal study. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2002; 82(6):993-1006. View

3.
Eisenberg N, Carlo G, Murphy B, Van Court P . Prosocial development in late adolescence: a longitudinal study. Child Dev. 1995; 66(4):1179-97. View

4.
Eisenberg N, Fabes R, Murphy B, Karbon M, Maszk P, Smith M . The relations of emotionality and regulation to dispositional and situational empathy-related responding. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1994; 66(4):776-97. DOI: 10.1037//0022-3514.66.4.776. View

5.
Williams B, Ponesse J, Schachar R, Logan G, Tannock R . Development of inhibitory control across the life span. Dev Psychol. 1999; 35(1):205-13. DOI: 10.1037//0012-1649.35.1.205. View