» Articles » PMID: 20537324

The Long and the Short of It: on the Nature and Origin of Functional Overlap Between Representations of Space and Time

Overview
Journal Cognition
Publisher Elsevier
Specialty Psychology
Date 2010 Jun 12
PMID 20537324
Citations 48
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

When we describe time, we often use the language of space (The movie was long; The deadline is approaching). Experiments 1-3 asked whether-as patterns in language suggest-a structural similarity between representations of spatial length and temporal duration is easier to access than one between length and other dimensions of experience, such as loudness. Adult participants were shown pairings of lines of different length with tones of different duration (Experiment 1) or tones of different loudness (Experiment 2). The length of the lines and duration or loudness of the tones was either positively or negatively correlated. Participants were better able to bind particular lengths and durations when they were positively correlated than when they were not, a pattern not observed for pairings of lengths and tone amplitudes, even after controlling for the presence of visual cues to duration in Experiment 1 (Experiment 3). This suggests that representations of length and duration may functionally overlap to a greater extent than representations of length and loudness. Experiments 4 and 5 asked whether experience with and mastery of words like long and short-which can flexibly refer to both space and time-itself creates this privileged relationship. Nine-month-old infants, like adults, were better able to bind representations of particular lengths and durations when these were positively correlated (Experiment 4), and failed to show this pattern for pairings of lengths and tone amplitudes (Experiment 5). We conclude that the functional overlap between representations of length and duration does not result from a metaphoric construction processes mediated by learning to flexibly use words such as long and short. We suggest instead that it may reflect an evolutionary recycling of spatial representations for more general purposes.

Citing Articles

Decomposing geographical judgments into spatial, temporal and linguistic components.

Gatti D, Anceresi G, Marelli M, Vecchi T, Rinaldi L Psychol Res. 2024; 88(5):1590-1601.

PMID: 38836875 PMC: 11282145. DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-01980-7.


Memory capacity as the core mechanism of the development of space-time interferences in children.

Hallez Q, Balci F Sci Rep. 2024; 14(1):10377.

PMID: 38710784 PMC: 11074329. DOI: 10.1038/s41598-024-61018-1.


Everything is Infinite: Children's Beliefs About Endless Space, Time, and Number.

Sullivan J, Cramer-Benjamin S, Alvarez J, Barner D Open Mind (Camb). 2023; 7:715-731.

PMID: 37840760 PMC: 10575555. DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00104.


Gradient Organization of Space, Time, and Numbers in the Brain: A Meta-analysis of Neuroimaging Studies.

Cona G, Wiener M, Allegrini F, Scarpazza C Neuropsychol Rev. 2023; 34(3):721-737.

PMID: 37594695 PMC: 11478975. DOI: 10.1007/s11065-023-09609-z.


Language as a mechanism for reasoning about possibilities.

Grigoroglou M, Ganea P Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2022; 377(1866):20210334.

PMID: 36314149 PMC: 9620752. DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2021.0334.


References
1.
Gallistel , Gelman I . Non-verbal numerical cognition: from reals to integers. Trends Cogn Sci. 2000; 4(2):59-65. DOI: 10.1016/s1364-6613(99)01424-2. View

2.
Boroditsky L . Metaphoric structuring: understanding time through spatial metaphors. Cognition. 2000; 75(1):1-28. DOI: 10.1016/s0010-0277(99)00073-6. View

3.
Boroditsky L . Does language shape thought? Mandarin and English speakers' conceptions of time. Cogn Psychol. 2001; 43(1):1-22. DOI: 10.1006/cogp.2001.0748. View

4.
Cohen Kadosh R, Cohen Kadosh K, Henik A . When brightness counts: the neuronal correlate of numerical-luminance interference. Cereb Cortex. 2007; 18(2):337-43. DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhm058. View

5.
Paivio A . Perceptual comparisons through the mind's eye. Mem Cognit. 2013; 3(6):635-47. DOI: 10.3758/BF03198229. View