» Articles » PMID: 20046219

Structure and Continuity of Intellectual Development in Early Childhood

Overview
Journal Intelligence
Publisher Elsevier
Date 2010 Jan 5
PMID 20046219
Citations 6
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

We evaluated over 200 participants semiannually from 12 to 48 months of age on measures of intellectual (Bayley Scales, Stanford-Binet Scale) and verbal (MacArthur-Bates Inventory, Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test) status. Structural equation modeling and hierarchical linear (growth curve) analyses were applied to address the nature of development and individual differences during this time. Structural analyses showed a strong and robust simplex model from infancy to the preschool period, with no evidence of qualitative reorganizations or discontinuities. Growth-curve modeling revealed significant associations between level factors across the early and later measures of cognition, providing further evidence of continuity; the growth trajectory from the Bayley through 24 months predicted growth in a nonverbal factor, but not in a verbal factor. Altogether, the findings reveal continuous and stable development in intellectual function from late infancy through the preschool years. Additionally, the high level of continuity demonstrated across these ages was observed to be largely independent of growth in vocabulary.

Citing Articles

Prerequisite Skills in Cognitive Testing: Innovations in theory and recommendations for practice.

Kaat A, Bishop S, Condy E, Sullivan N, Soorya L, Thurm A Cogn Dev. 2021; 58.

PMID: 33833479 PMC: 8023649. DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2021.101038.


A longitudinal study of higher-order thinking skills: working memory and fluid reasoning in childhood enhance complex problem solving in adolescence.

Greiff S, Wustenberg S, Goetz T, Vainikainen M, Hautamaki J, Bornstein M Front Psychol. 2015; 6:1060.

PMID: 26283992 PMC: 4515543. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01060.


Human infancy…and the rest of the lifespan.

Bornstein M Annu Rev Psychol. 2014; 65:121-58.

PMID: 24405360 PMC: 5865600. DOI: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100359.


Multicenter study of neurodevelopment in 3-year-old children with and without single-suture craniosynostosis.

Starr J, Collett B, Gaither R, Kapp-Simon K, Cradock M, Cunningham M Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2012; 166(6):536-42.

PMID: 22312170 PMC: 3407295. DOI: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2011.1800.


Information Processing in Toddlers: Continuity from Infancy and Persistence of Preterm Deficits.

Rose S, Feldman J, Jankowski J Intelligence. 2010; 37(3):311-320.

PMID: 20161244 PMC: 2706531. DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2009.02.002.


References
1.
Craik F, Bialystok E . Cognition through the lifespan: mechanisms of change. Trends Cogn Sci. 2006; 10(3):131-8. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.01.007. View

2.
McCall R, Hogarty P, Hurlburt N . Transitions in infant sensorimotor development and the prediction of childhood IQ. Am Psychol. 1972; 27(8):728-48. DOI: 10.1037/h0033148. View

3.
Lewis M . Infant intelligence tests: their use and misuse. Hum Dev. 1973; 16(1):108-18. DOI: 10.1159/000271270. View

4.
Siegel L . Reproductive, perinatal, and environmental factors as predictors of the cognitive and language development of preterm and full-term infants. Child Dev. 1982; 53(4):963-73. View

5.
Fenson L, Dale P, Reznick J, Bates E, Thal D, Pethick S . Variability in early communicative development. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 1994; 59(5):1-173; discussion 174-85. View