» Articles » PMID: 18267005

Phonological Processing of Ignored Distractor Pictures, an FMRI Investigation

Overview
Journal BMC Neurosci
Publisher Biomed Central
Specialty Neurology
Date 2008 Feb 13
PMID 18267005
Citations 21
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Background: Neuroimaging studies of attention often focus on interactions between stimulus representations and top-down selection mechanisms in visual cortex. Less is known about the neural representation of distractor stimuli beyond visual areas, and the interactions between stimuli in linguistic processing areas. In the present study, participants viewed simultaneously presented line drawings at peripheral locations, while in the MRI scanner. The names of the objects depicted in these pictures were either phonologically related (i.e. shared the same consonant-vowel onset construction), or unrelated. Attention was directed either at the linguistic properties of one of these pictures, or at the fixation point (i.e. away from the pictures).

Results: Phonological representations of unattended pictures could be detected in the posterior superior temporal gyrus, the inferior frontal gyrus, and the insula.

Conclusion: Under some circumstances, the name of ignored distractor pictures is retrieved by linguistic areas. This implies that selective attention to a specific location does not completely filter out the representations of distractor stimuli at early perceptual stages.

Citing Articles

Activating words without language: beta and theta oscillations reflect lexical access and control processes during verbal and non-verbal object recognition tasks.

Branzi F, Martin C, Biau E Cereb Cortex. 2023; 33(10):6228-6240.

PMID: 36724048 PMC: 10183750. DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhac499.


The Time Course of Language Production as Revealed by Pattern Classification of MEG Sensor Data.

Carota F, Schoffelen J, Oostenveld R, Indefrey P J Neurosci. 2022; 42(29):5745-5754.

PMID: 35680410 PMC: 9302460. DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1923-21.2022.


Language is activated by visual input regardless of memory demands or capacity.

Chabal S, Hayakawa S, Marian V Cognition. 2022; 222:104994.

PMID: 35016119 PMC: 8898262. DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104994.


Task-Relevant Representations and Cognitive Control Demands Modulate Functional Connectivity from Ventral Occipito-Temporal Cortex During Object Recognition Tasks.

Branzi F, Martin C, Paz-Alonso P Cereb Cortex. 2021; 32(14):3068-3080.

PMID: 34918042 PMC: 9290561. DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhab401.


Intracranial recording in patients with aphasia using nanomaterial-based flexible electronics: promises and challenges.

Wang Q, Siok W Beilstein J Nanotechnol. 2021; 12:330-342.

PMID: 33889479 PMC: 8042484. DOI: 10.3762/bjnano.12.27.


References
1.
Di Lollo V, Dixon P . Two forms of persistence in visual information processing. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1988; 14(4):671-681. DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.14.4.671. View

2.
Bradshaw J . Peripherally presented and unreported words may bias the perceived meaning of a centrally fixated homograph. J Exp Psychol. 1974; 103(6):1200-2. DOI: 10.1037/h0037371. View

3.
van Turennout M, Hagoort P, Brown C . Electrophysiological evidence on the time course of semantic and phonological processes in speech production. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 1997; 23(4):787-806. DOI: 10.1037//0278-7393.23.4.787. View

4.
Fiez J . Phonology, semantics, and the role of the left inferior prefrontal cortex. Hum Brain Mapp. 1997; 5(2):79-83. View

5.
Borowsky R, Cummine J, Owen W, Friesen C, Shih F, Sarty G . FMRI of ventral and dorsal processing streams in basic reading processes: insular sensitivity to phonology. Brain Topogr. 2006; 18(4):233-9. DOI: 10.1007/s10548-006-0001-2. View