» Articles » PMID: 18094748

Perceptual Learning in the Absence of Task or Stimulus Specificity

Overview
Journal PLoS One
Date 2007 Dec 21
PMID 18094748
Citations 14
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Performance on most sensory tasks improves with practice. When making particularly challenging sensory judgments, perceptual improvements in performance are tightly coupled to the trained task and stimulus configuration. The form of this specificity is believed to provide a strong indication of which neurons are solving the task or encoding the learned stimulus. Here we systematically decouple task- and stimulus-mediated components of trained improvements in perceptual performance and show that neither provides an adequate description of the learning process. Twenty-four human subjects trained on a unique combination of task (three-element alignment or bisection) and stimulus configuration (vertical or horizontal orientation). Before and after training, we measured subjects' performance on all four task-configuration combinations. What we demonstrate for the first time is that learning does actually transfer across both task and configuration provided there is a common spatial axis to the judgment. The critical factor underlying the transfer of learning effects is not the task or stimulus arrangements themselves, but rather the recruitment of commons sets of neurons most informative for making each perceptual judgment.

Citing Articles

Speech in noise perception improved by training fine auditory discrimination: far and applicable transfer of perceptual learning.

Gao X, Yan T, Huang T, Li X, Zhang Y Sci Rep. 2020; 10(1):19320.

PMID: 33168921 PMC: 7653913. DOI: 10.1038/s41598-020-76295-9.


Performance-monitoring integrated reweighting model of perceptual learning.

Sotiropoulos G, Seitz A, Series P Vision Res. 2018; 152:17-39.

PMID: 29581060 PMC: 6200663. DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2018.01.010.


Auditory Discrimination Learning: Role of Working Memory.

Zhang Y, Moore D, Guiraud J, Molloy K, Yan T, Amitay S PLoS One. 2016; 11(1):e0147320.

PMID: 26799068 PMC: 4723131. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0147320.


Confidence-based integrated reweighting model of task-difficulty explains location-based specificity in perceptual learning.

Talluri B, Hung S, Seitz A, Series P J Vis. 2016; 15(10):17.

PMID: 26720153 PMC: 4699478. DOI: 10.1167/15.10.17.


The challenges of developing a contrast-based video game for treatment of amblyopia.

Hussain Z, Astle A, Webb B, McGraw P Front Psychol. 2014; 5:1210.

PMID: 25404922 PMC: 4217344. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01210.


References
1.
Karni A, Sagi D . Where practice makes perfect in texture discrimination: evidence for primary visual cortex plasticity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1991; 88(11):4966-70. PMC: 51788. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.88.11.4966. View

2.
Wilson D, Fletcher M, Sullivan R . Acetylcholine and olfactory perceptual learning. Learn Mem. 2004; 11(1):28-34. PMC: 1868532. DOI: 10.1101/lm.66404. View

3.
Crist R, Kapadia M, Westheimer G, Gilbert C . Perceptual learning of spatial localization: specificity for orientation, position, and context. J Neurophysiol. 1998; 78(6):2889-94. DOI: 10.1152/jn.1997.78.6.2889. View

4.
Fahle M, Edelman S, Poggio T . Fast perceptual learning in hyperacuity. Vision Res. 1995; 35(21):3003-13. DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(95)00044-z. View

5.
Ahissar M, Hochstein S . Learning pop-out detection: specificities to stimulus characteristics. Vision Res. 1996; 36(21):3487-500. DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(96)00036-3. View