» Articles » PMID: 16817008

Do Provocateurs' Emotion Displays Influence Children's Social Goals and Problem Solving?

Overview
Publisher Springer
Date 2006 Jul 4
PMID 16817008
Citations 3
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

The social goals and social problem-solving of children who varied in social adjustment were examined in the context of hypothetical ambiguous provocation situations in which provocateurs' emotion displays were systematically manipulated. Children rated the importance of six different social goals and explained how they would solve the problems. Social adjustment was measured with rating and nomination sociometric procedures. Rejected-aggressive, rejected-nonaggressive, average-nonaggressive, and popular-nonaggressive children showed both commonalities and differences in rating the six social goals, the relative importance of the six social goals, and social problem-solving depending on the provocateur's emotion display. When provocateurs were happy, there were few group differences, but when provocateurs were angry or sad, rejected-aggressive children: a) rated hostile/instrumental goals more positively; b) rated prosocial goals less positively; and c) made problem-solving responses that were less friendly than those of other children. Results are discussed in relation to Lemerise and Arsenio's (2000) model of emotion and social information processing.

Citing Articles

Development and validation of the social information processing application: a Web-based measure of social information processing patterns in elementary school-age boys.

Kupersmidt J, Stelter R, Dodge K Psychol Assess. 2011; 23(4):834-47.

PMID: 21534693 PMC: 3715038. DOI: 10.1037/a0023621.


Children's representations of family relationships, peer information processing, and school adjustment.

Bascoe S, Davies P, Sturge-Apple M, Cummings E Dev Psychol. 2009; 45(6):1740-51.

PMID: 19899928 PMC: 2912155. DOI: 10.1037/a0016688.


The effect of induced mood on children's social information processing: goal clarification and response decision.

Harper B, Lemerise E, Caverly S J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2009; 38(5):575-86.

PMID: 19763815 DOI: 10.1007/s10802-009-9356-7.

References
1.
Grotpeter J, Crick N . Relational aggression, overt aggression, and friendship. Child Dev. 1996; 67(5):2328-38. View

2.
Crick N, Dodge K . Social information-processing mechanisms in reactive and proactive aggression. Child Dev. 1996; 67(3):993-1002. View

3.
Erdley C, Asher S . Children's social goals and self-efficacy perceptions as influences on their responses to ambiguous provocation. Child Dev. 1996; 67(4):1329-44. View

4.
Orobio de Castro B, Veerman J, Koops W, Bosch J, Monshouwer H . Hostile attribution of intent and aggressive behavior: a meta-analysis. Child Dev. 2002; 73(3):916-34. DOI: 10.1111/1467-8624.00447. View

5.
Bagwell C, Coie J . The best friendships of aggressive boys: relationship quality, conflict management, and rule-breaking behavior. J Exp Child Psychol. 2004; 88(1):5-24. DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2003.11.004. View